‘Text’-book ?

I am no longer on a campus and won’t do the evangelism task of OpenEdMOOC week 3. However, I will at least think about what would be the most urgent cases to address.

There is a wide range of cases for open access, from the one extreme where a publisher just steps in between author and readers and demands money for no service, right through to cases where a large work is assembled through the coordination of many authors including perpetual improvements and enhancements. Textbooks often fall into the latter category, and they are explicitly excluded from the barrier regulations limiting copyright in favor of science and education in Germany.

An important occasion to copy from a textbook is when it contains a great instructional graphic. (Despite the English term ‘text’-book suggests that the text is more important; but in German it is called the equivalent of ‘teach-book’.)

From J. M. Flagg’s famous Uncle Sam poster, http://www.loc.gov/pictures/resource/ppmsca.50554/

An image says more than 1000 words, and the careful selection of what to show and how to highlight the salient features, is certainly a great intellectual merit. Which deserves credit. But what is protected by copyright is not this idea (at least over here) but merely the ‘work’.

So, what if we take the idea for the picture and draw a new picture, modelled after the antetype of the given one? (Of course not just by following all contours through a sandwich paper or foil.) And then attribute the creator of the original image? According to my legal understanding (which is, of course, not professional) this should comply with the requirements that the work is commercially protected while the idea needs scholarly attribution and credit.

But it is not easy to do such drawings. I tried to redraw some outline of J. M. Flagg’s famous Uncle Sam picture, and miserably failed. (On this occasion I learned that also Flaggs copied the idea from a British precursor).

Posted in OpenEdMOOC | Leave a comment

The trap of copyright

I liked Wiley’s description of the current copyright situation as a trap (“it can feel really trapping”, 8.24′). Much of what sounds intuitively reasonable is prohibited when a copy of knowledge is treated as economic ‘property’ despite it just doesn’t fit this category in many ways (e.g. the copy is no longer a scarce value, and you can’t inspect it before purchase, you cannot return it and ‘unknow’ it, and so on).

Photo: Trap by Flickr user aydun cc-by

In practice, many try to ignore this trap. For example, when they use the ‘work’ of a textbook image in their course, they often confuse quotation rights with work use rights: if they don’t “critically engage with” the quoted part, they don’t qualify for the former kind of right. Which is meant to apply to an idea which is usually not protected against exploitation but to be tracked for credit and validity. By ignoring this, they won’t notice just how much the trap impedes their teaching even behind a Moodle wall.

Others try to argue ideologically or emotionally, by equating all IP: the rights of a small painter (who makes a living with his works) to the work of a tenured person who writes their books “on their own time” — which nobody dares to critically distinguish from his total time outside the classroom.

But most authors seem to put up with the trap. The prestigious journal is dictator, and they have learned (during their many years as adjunct slaves) that resistance is futile? Wiley has great responses to such kind of senior faculty:

“the only people tying us to the old system is us.”

and

“science advances one funeral at a time.”

 

Posted in OpenEdMOOC | 1 Comment

OpenEdMOOC Test

Testing as recommended on this page:

Course  > Week 1: Why Open Matters  > Getting Ready for the Course  > Course Activities Overview

Posted in OpenEdMOOC | 2 Comments

Small differences

I have long admired how iMapping uses small differences to make a big change in the User Experience of this interesting think tool.

For example, its “targeted zooming”: If you happen to remember where a desired detail is located on their big overview map, you can directly click it. This does not seem like a big deal because we are used to zooming through several zoom levels and we don’t mind this, really, because we don’t think about the additional steps any more. But each step increases the danger of being distracted by the patronizing user interface — and adds friction to the mind’s operation.

(On the way back, then, I might prefer the step-by-step navigation which is also offered, via Esc, to avoid the ‘sea-sick’ effect known from Prezi.)

Now they have introduced a new export format that has a similar (small & beneficial) effect (and this is indirectly also available to my condensr users who export to iMapping): “Setevi” = Semantic Text View. It allows to selectively expand or collapse the individual items, according to the user’s wishes (personal, not just personalized by the system). One might think: so what — didn’t Dave Winer’s Fargo.io do just that long ago? But the small difference is that Setevi conserves as much space as possible. And so, even a page with many expanded levels won’t get too long — which, in turn, makes it easier to discover similarities and hidden conceptual connections.

(Furthermore, the affordance of personal adjustment within a page, reminds me of the idea of Hyperscope that was derived from Doug Engelbart’s later work.)

Unfortunately, such small differences don’t seem to get much attention from empirical academic studies. Probably, the risk is too high that they won’t yield a neat success within the time-span of a thesis or funded project?

Posted in Usability | Leave a comment

Mental flexors and extensors

Cathleen MacDonald of @divided_brain asks

“we’re creating a free brain health quiz to raise awareness/educate. What issues need attention?”

and my response does not fit in 140 characters:

I trust that this quiz will not become just a personality self-test (“Are you left brained or right-brained?”) like those that entertained us in the 60s but probably damaged the reputation of any consideration of styles, for decades. And I hope it will not be just a test with “true or false” questions and with the goal of accumulating a maximal number of scores — because such an activity would onty cater to the one mode of operation that McGilchrist compares to a bird’s picking of a grain, using narrow focussed attention.

Rather, the attention involved here, should resemble his opposite comparison: an animal’s broad vigilant attention to detect a predator among twigs in the twilight. So, perhaps the linear list of quiz questions could be enriched by some broader, laminar, picture tasks? Such as a ‘busy scenes’ picture (like “Where is Waldo”), or a task with concepts to be connected or rearranged (like my condensr), or an exploration of some dispersed items (like the doors of an Advent calendar) ?

To spot the ‘predator’ of mental health, it is probably not useful to watch out for the extreme symptoms of an unhealthy unbalance of the two modes. Rather, it is necessary to understand how each of these two modes works in the normal, day to day, operation.

Image: Wikipedia

In my view, McGilchrist’s “Master” and “Emissary” modes are the mind’s equivalent of what bending and stretching are for the body: they are both involved in almost every activity, and if they are not in balance, we fall.

So we need to understand the ‘flexors’ and ‘extensors’ of brain operation, and we need to identify their respective contribution to the balance in everyday behaviors.

Posted in Cognitive Styles | 3 Comments

Tools vs. practice

Jim McGee wrote a great piece on tools and practices. He argues that the ease of getting started with the first 5% is deceptive.

“We focus on the details of particular features and functions at the expense of ignoring the cognitive challenges of deep thought and collaborative work.”

I think it is important that we distinguish between “deep thought and collaborative work”. Tools for these two coincide only for roughly 5%.

It is useful, though, to recall that these 5% did have their merit. Almost 10 years ago, I wrote for my users (in German, here,) “What does my personal productivity have to do with ‘social’ software tools? … users use the same tools [for both].” (In particular, social bookmarks and annotations thrived because they were selfishly provided but were useful to others, and on the other hand, blogs and wikis often served also as notes to self.)

Also, I would like to defend the ease of getting started. Trying out a new tool is a risky investment of time. But there is no way of evaluating a think tool without personally trying it out. The closer it is to the mind, the more it can be compared to a tight garment which must perfectly fit.

For ‘deep thinking’, tools need to be more than just logistics (storage and transport) of thoughts. (In his recent blog post on Intelligence Augmentation, Clark Quinn tackled the tight integration of the ‘cognitive and computational architectures’ by carefully carving out some aspects of what the latter can do well — details, repetition, find correlations.)

Now for collaboration, the ‘tight garment’ cannot fit multiple persons simultaneously. A sad practice is often that the group settles on the least common denominator (email) because a stubborn member (the boss?) refuses to try out a better way.

Here it is useful to distinguish between passive acceptance/ refusal, and active choice of one’s pet function. And then apply Postel’s Principle: “Be conservative in what you send, be liberal in what you accept“. For example, messages without subject line or with the repeated, misleading subject line, are read but should still be avoided. The trick is then to try out what range of choices are tolerable for everyone to accept — not only which features optimally cater to individual expressiveness.

In practice, important examples of differing tastes are: How much should be sent across ‘push’ channels as opposed to ‘pull’ channels, or: where is more divergent information appropriate and where should convergent contributions prevail. Often, the intervention of a stout-hearted but gentle personality such as the ‘wiki gardener’ is needed. So, to answer Jim’s question “Where are good places to look?“: in gardening 🙂 His guess, software development, does not sound too promising to me — despite the GitHub workflow appears like magic — because tt requires more discipline than is usually accepted.

Posted in Knowledge management | Leave a comment

Polarization and Push or Pull

Great food for thought in @gsiemens and @realdlnorman’s exchange. So why can’t media be like a conversation? What makes the internet a “polarization factory” of ‘us vs. them’?

I think the problem is that is has turned too much towards ‘push’ media, and that these are prone to scale-free effects, with their potentially unlimited reach. Each statement uttered in this environment, is soon understood as advocating for a cause or for a political strand, because everyone seems to expect that their tweet may go viral, because that is how the platforms work. Then a differentiated statement can be ripped out of its context, and the ‘yes, but’ is distorted to just ‘no’.

By contrast, ‘pull’ media such as the comment section of a personal blog, resemble the conversation that George describes. The visitor on the blogger’s ‘front porch’ has bothered to come there, and sees the context and the background of a position.

(And yes, I will have to ‘push’ this URL out on Twitter, because RSS is almost dead. So there is no ‘them’ who I can blame for the “polarization factory”; it’s all of ‘us’ who operate it.)

Posted in Web | 3 Comments

Distinctively human?

1. In his latest presentation, Stephen Downes once more advocated personal learning. Previously I have often wondered if it is just a matter of style and taste whether one emphasizes an individual access to learning or not, i.e., whether one prefers learning in the same way a backpacker individual tourist learns about the world, or if one finds it easier and more effective to go with a guided group tour to get the proven optimized standard sight.

 Photo CC-BY by Flickr user supermoving

As a tourist, I never liked the group style, but for learning, the lazy way of letting stuff just wash over me, did have its charme. And it sounds even more promising now since the optimization is ever more perfected by personalization. And since it seems like a ‘science-based’ way to success.

2. There may be even a third type of learners: the machine ‘students’ who do their machine learning. Previously, I often wondered why machines needed to ‘learn’, at all: they don’t need to gradually accumulate their knowledge because they just do a database lookup; they don’t have to learn their rules because they receive them through programming; and learning for any kind of later independence seemed unimaginable.

Now I know that it is not rules, but patterns, what they learn: they learn from data that comes in large amounts and from data that is randomly distributed — much like lifelong human experiences. So our automated fellow learners are already far away from the group path, and they are rapidly approaching our human styles.

3. In an Australian conference last week, @SBuckShum talked about the Cognitive Automation and recommended that we

“Cultivate those qualities that are distinctively human”

So what will prove as ‘distinctively human’ ?

I think it is exactly the personal approach which cannot be standardized, that cannot be automated. It is the individuality and subjectivity that guarantees sufficient diversity for further evolution and to avoid collapsing into a ‘black hole’ of power law distributions, and that guarantees sufficient embodiment for staying grounded in reality.

By contrast, approaches that only accept objectivity, and strive for algorithms that promise an optimized, rational, assessable, uniform solution — are eligible for cognitive automation, sooner or later.

4. This also applies to the optimization of learning. Only standardized, easily assessable learning can be optimized, and in particular, content knowledge is the ideal example. If we compare learning to carrying a load upstairs to the attic storage, there are two distinct goals. One is that the load should be stored up there, and the other is to work out one’s muscles. If it is only necessary that the stuff is accumulated up there in our brain, a ‘lift’ would be a welcome optimization. But jobs that only rely on such content storage, are probably the first to be eliminated.

In a recent blog post, Stephen calls it an ‘either or’ dichotomy:

“are you providing content knowledge or, are you providing literacies?”

and I think it it really a shame how ignorant our universities are in dealing with this question.

Posted in Knowledge | Leave a comment

Seven habits of…

The seven habits of successful people? Well, yes, except that it is only one person (me), and ‘successful’ just means that I am highly content with the workflow that I am going to describe.

  1. I don’t open my email if I could not react, anyway.
  2. I prefer RSS over email news.
  3. I store an item whenever I have reflected on it.
  4. I don’t care much while capturing stuff,
  5. But I care a lot for grouping it later.
  6. Of course, I use my Thought condensr.de think tool.
  7. I take an easy way to prune stuff.

Read more:

1. I don’t open my email if I could not react, anyway.

If I am preoccupied with a larger piece of work, or if I am not near my desk, a newly arrived email must wait. My curiosity about it is smaller than the reluctance to get interrupted. Enjoying an email means for me, to start thinking about a response rightaway, or to take the time to follow its scents.

2. I prefer RSS over email news.

My categorized RSS subscriptions come across to me as offerings rather than urges. They give me a sense of space, as if I was visiting the authors on their respective front porches, and I have already a certain idea of what awaits me there. Thus it is easier to notice the salient, unexpected, relevant news to pick.

Picking from RSS is more like ‘pull’ than ‘push’. It does not urge me to almost synchronously react — not even to decide if I delete an item or keep it or where to keep it: it rests on its web site without bothering me. Push vs. pull is a big issue today where lots of sites try to control our attention and patronize us. Joi Ito’s “Whiplash” has a whole chapter about pull over push.

Other kinds of notifications feel like a terribly linear assembly line. Once I get into such an unstructured linear list, I feel that it will mean a long time-span with almost no chance to interrupt and digress and still find my way back, and this is somehow daunting. ‘The stream’ is an ineffective construct just made to lure us into filter recommendations. It eats up power to connect items to their contexts rather than to relevant similar ones. Small bloggers are just ‘flushed’ down the stream. See also what Mike Caulfield writes about the stream.

3. I store an item whenever I have reflected on it.

Sometimes I am not sure if I have stored something in my ‘knowledge base’ of my harddisk. Then this little simple rule of thumb helps: If I have thought for some time about an item, I will store its url or a note. Even if it turned out to be rubbish (which I might note in brackets in the file name), I will vaguely remember it as somehow relevant and then I might try to find it again, so it is better to save a cue rather than search it in vain.

4. I don’t care much while capturing stuff.

Capturing an idea must be quick rather than optimized, to minimize the distraction from the task at hand. I don’t think about what would be the most appropriate medium for the note. I fire up Notepad and store the idea in a single separate file. I don’t think about the filename either, and I abbreviate ruthlessly. For my idea notes, the names are almost like a ‘private language’: typically, such a note is a one- or two-liner saying “xxxxxxxx has something to do with yyyyyyyy?”, and then the filename will be xxxxYyyy.txt which I will forget before I revisit my notes.

And I don’t care too much about the folder names. My filing is not optimized for directly searching, but for browsing in context and seeing what is there. So the folder names just need to be recognized within the context of the containing folder. Whenever I was looking in vain for something in one folder, I create a folder shortcut there, pointing me to the other folder next time. So over time, a dense network emerges, the traversing of which strengthens my awareness of the relationships.

And it is not important to find the optimized procedure having the shortest click path. Rather, a default procedure that is well-rehearsed will do to minimize distraction. So, my idea note files are saved to the desktop and dropped to a folder called ‘new’ — via a folder shortcut on the desktop. This minimally invasive note-taking guarantees that no tiny humble idea gets ignored and can be processed later — see next section.

5. But I care a lot for grouping it later.

In contrast to the careless capturing, I really cultivate my notes collection later. The crucial thing for me is not to classify them prematurely, but to be open for new similarities, connections, and patterns, and for new emerging categories becoming necessary. Once I put the notes into the disjoint categories of my private localhost WordPress, I have shuffled them around for so long that I know them well, and I rarely need cross-links.

6. Of course, I use my Thought condensr.de think tool

Whenever I start to produce some output, I drop all pertinent stuff into my condensr.de which helps me with the step between stuff collection and outline.

7. I take an easy way to prune stuff.

Sometimes I notice that a digression needs pruning, only after I have struggled quite a while with a difficult text, and I would not want to delete it entirely. Then I am glad about this well-rehearsed habit: I cut the text and paste it into a little text file called xxxxYyyy+1.txt — plus 1, for some distant next attempt that may never happen.

Posted in Personal Productivity | Leave a comment

Week 1 of new cMOOC

Finally I was accepted at the Federica.EU platform for the new cMOOC “Connectivism and Learning”. (Two earlier attempts failed even though I clicked on “I did not receive activation email”, and their support email address did not (yet?) work. But now the activation email was successfully received in my spam folder.) On the ‘plattaforma’, I did not find anything except Downes’s excellent videos and slides, but this should not be a problem in a cMOOC with blogs and all that. And the only remaining problem, the missing hashtag, can be added later on. So let’s start.

plattaforma

Stephen offered plenty of great questions for reflection, e.g. “Reflecting on your own learning, can you identify a process that you currently follow?” or “Do you think you could improve your learning if you had a better understanding of how you learn?” These questions remind me that I wanted to compile some notes about my workflows, hopefully soon.

Another question was about interactivity:

“Why do we need it ? […] second, we need to know that other people are in the learning experience with us.”

IMHO, this includes that we are watching our co-learners in their stage of recognizing, which is much more similar to the recognizing that we need to do ourselves, than the settled, packaged, resources presented by a traditional teacher. If the co-learners ‘teach’ us what they have just come to understand, they are ‘modeling and demonstrating’ their own understanding process.

And watching our co-humans, puts us automatically in a mode which is more open to immediate, contextual, multipoint, ‘all-at-once’ experience than consuming a slice of canned information. Frankly, I was long sceptical about the value of ‘social’ learning, and I suspected that it just helps those who are not willing to interact directly with the resources, and (of course) benefits those who repeat the stuff by explaining it to the weaker students. But in the meantime I have understood that there is more to it.

Posted in Learning | 3 Comments